Journalists are the archaeologist By Prof Dr Sohail Ansari & Simpson’s paradox.
Love yourself first,
because that is who you will be spending the rest of your life with. Unknown. “Husbands should take
good care of their wives, with [the bounties] God has given to some more than
others and with what they spend out of their own money. Righteous wives are
devout and guard what God would have them guard in their husbands’ absence…” [Qur’an, 4.34]
Journalists do not waste time with their own explanations
·
Journalists are in the search for the explanations people want
to hear.
Henry
"Indiana" Jones, Jr
·
Archeology is the
search for facts, not truth. If it's truth you're looking for, Dr. Tyree's
philosophy class is right down the hall
·
Don't waste your time with explanations: people only hear what they
want to hear. Paulo Coelho
Simpson’s paradox
What is
it?
This is where trends that appear within different groups
disappear when data for those groups are combined. When this happens, the
overall trend might even appear to be the opposite of the trends in each group.
One example of this paradox is where a treatment can be
detrimental in all groups of patients, yet can appear beneficial overall once
the groups are combined.
How
does it happen?
This can happen when the sizes of the groups are uneven. A trial
with careless (or unscrupulous) selection of the numbers of patients could
conclude that a harmful treatment appears beneficial.
Example
Consider the following double
blind trial of a proposed medical treatment. A group of 120 patients (split
into subgroups of sizes 10, 20, 30 and 60) receive the treatment, and 120
patients (split into subgroups of corresponding sizes 60, 30, 20 and 10)
receive no treatment.
The overall results make it look like the treatment was
beneficial to patients, with a higher recovery rate for patients with the
treatment than for those without it.
However,
when you drill down into the various groups that made up the cohort in the
study, you see in all groups of patients, the recovery rate was 50% higher for
patients who had no treatment.
But note that the size and age distribution of each group is
different between those who took the treatment and those who didn’t. This is
what distorts the numbers. In this case, the treatment group is
disproportionately stacked with children, whose recovery rates are typically
higher, with or without treatment.
Base
rate fallacy
What is
it?
This fallacy occurs when we disregard important information when
making a judgement on how likely something is.
If, for
example, we hear that someone loves music, we might think it’s more
likely they’re a professional musician than an accountant. However, there are
many more accountants than there are professional musicians. Here we have
neglected that the base
rate for the number of accountants is far higher than the number
of musicians, so we were unduly swayed by the information that the person
likes music.
How does
it happen?
The base rate fallacy occurs when the base rate for one option
is substantially higher than for another.
Example
Consider testing for a rare medical condition, such as one that
affects only 4% (1 in 25) of a population.
Let’s say
there is a test for the condition, but it’s not perfect. If someone has the
condition, the test will correctly identify them as being ill around 92% of the
time. If someone doesn’t have
the condition, the test will correctly identify them as being healthy 75% of
the time.
So if we test a group of people, and find that over a quarter of
them are diagnosed as being ill, we might expect that most of these people
really do have the condition. But we’d be wrong.
In a typical sample of 300
patients, for every 11 people correctly identified as unwell, a further 72 are
incorrectly identified as unwell. The Conversation, CC BY-ND
According
to our numbers above, of the 4% of patients who are ill, almost 92% will be
correctly diagnosed as ill (that is, about 3.67% of the overall population).
But of the 96% of patients who are not ill, 25% will be incorrectly diagnosed as ill
(that’s 24% of the overall population).
What this means is that of the approximately 27.67% of the
population who are diagnosed as ill, only around 3.67% actually are. So of the
people who were diagnosed as ill, only around 13% (that is, 3.67%/27.67%) actually
are unwell.
Worryingly, when a famous
study asked general practitioners to perform a similar calculation
to inform patients of the correct risks associated with mammogram results, just
15% of them did so correctly.
Comments
Post a Comment